Nature 410, 324 - 325 (2001)
© Macmillan Publishers Ltd.
Palaeoanthropology: Did our ancestors knuckle-walk?
MIKE DAINTON
Department of Human Anatomy
and Cell Biology, New Medical School, University of Liverpool,
Ashton Street, Liverpool L69 3GE, UK
e-mail: mdainton@liverpool.ac.uk
All African apes walk on
their knuckles. There is no evidence for this behaviour in the
earliest hominids, however, which conflicts with molecular DNA
evidence suggesting that chimpanzees are more closely related
to humans than to gorillas. On the basis of a multivariate analysis
of four traits of the proximal wrist joint, Richmond and Strait1
claim that African apes and early hominids do share a common knuckle-walking
ancestor. I propose that these traits are not uniquely associated
with knuckle-walking and question the basis of their conclusion.
It is still possible that no human ancestor knuckle-walked and
that this behaviour evolved independently in different species.
Although such an ancestor
would counter objections to an exclusive humanchimpanzee
clade, it would not prove that knuckle-walking evolved only once
in the ancestry of African apes and humans. Richmond and Strait1
discount the possibility that knuckle-walking evolved independently
in gorillas and in a common humanchimpanzee ancestor, an
idea supported by the different ontogenetic development of gorilla
and chimpanzee carpus load-bearing morphology2 and by terrestrial
orang-utans who normally walk on the dorsum of their fingers,
albeit in a less efficient3 fist-walking posture4, 5, and occasionally
knuckle-walk6, 7. The entire great-ape lineage may thus be predisposed
towards knuckle-walking when terrestrial quadrupedalism becomes
a major selective factor6, 7, undermining the claim that the four
cited traits can resolve the phylogenetic relationships of chimpanzees,
humans and gorillas1.
Richmond and Strait1 assume
that because some features of the present-day distal radius fulfil
one of the mechanical requisites of African-ape knuckle-walking
(limiting extension at the radiocarpal joint), then they must
have arisen in response to the requirements of knuckle-walking.
But it is debatable whether form, function and adaptive significance
should be so linked within a modern evolutionary framework8, 9.
Claims for knuckle-walking adaptations in the wrist need careful
evaluation as this behaviour describes a novel posture of the
metacarpals and phalanges in relation to a substrate4, 5. Digitigrade,
semidigitigrade or fist-walking postures may make similar demands
on the wrist joints to knuckle-walking4 and so could also have
given rise to stabilizing and extension-limiting mechanisms at
the radiocarpal joint.
Extension-limiting structures
allow contact between the hand and substrate to be maintained
more efficiently when climbing large vertical supports4 and so
may have arisen in a climbing ancestor of humans and great apes10.
>From this perspective, Richmond and Strait1 only demonstrate
that behaviours requiring limited extension at the radiocarpal
joint were important in hominid ancestry. Methodological considerations,
however, suggest that the proposed extension-limiting mechanism
of the distal radius may not have been suitably quantified.
A clearly delimited scaphoid
notch is not always evident in the hominoid species included in
the analysis1, so the measurements may not be replicable. It is
unclear why the angle between the scaphoid and lunate facets of
the radius may be significant in quantifying the extension-limiting
mechanisms at this joint. This dimension should discriminate between
African and Asian apes, with a narrower angle in the latter. The
greater radio-ulnar curvature of the distal radial articulation
in orang-utans and gibbons is atypical for anthropoid primates,
and probably facilitates the rotary movement of the proximal carpal
joint needed for forearm suspensory locomotion4, 11. But it is
not explained why increased rotary movement should preclude an
extension-limiting mechanism at the radiocarpal joint1. Part of
the discrimination between African apes, Asian apes and hominids
(their Fig. 2) may therefore reflect differences in suspensory
behaviour and have nothing to do with knuckle-walking.
The first two canonical axes
of Fig. 2b of ref. 1 do not separate the A. afarensis radii from
orang-utans any more than from chimpanzees. The variables that
strongly contribute to the remaining axis are not identified,
so it is unclear why the Mahalonobis D2 distance is greater between
orang-utans and A. afarensis than between chimpanzees and A. afarensis
(their Fig. 2c, based on all canonical axes), a difference that
bears on the conclusion that A. afarensis had a knuckle-walking
ancestry.
Although hominids may have
evolved from a knuckle-walker, I do not believe that Richmond
and Strait's analysis1 has proved this. I cannot, therefore, support
related interpretations regarding the phylogenetic affinity1 and
locomotor mode12 of early hominids, or of the chimpanzee-like
characteristics of their immediate ancestor1.
References
1. Richmond, B. G. & Strait, D. S. Nature 404, 382-385 (2000).
| Article | PubMed |
2. Dainton, M. & Macho, G. A. J. Hum. Evol. 36, 171-194 (1999).
| Article | PubMed |
3. Tuttle, R. H. J. Morphol. 128, 309-364 (1969). | PubMed |
4. Sarmiento, E. E. Int. J. Primatol. 9, 281-345 (1988).
5. Tuttle, R. H. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 26, 171-206 (1967).
6. Tuttle, R. H. (ed.) in Primate Functional Morphology and Evolution
203-212 (Mouton, The Hague, 1975).
7. Tuttle, R. H. & Beck, B. B. Nature 236, 33-44 (1972). |
PubMed |
8. Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. S. Paleobiology 8, 4-15 (1982).
9. Lauder, G. V. in Functional Morphology in Vertebrate Paleontology
(ed. Thomason, J. J.) 1-18 (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge,
1995).
10. Sarmiento, E. E. Hum. Evol. 10, 289-321 (1995).
11. Jenkins, F. A.Jr & Fleagle, J. G. in Primate Functional
Morphology and Evolution (ed. Tuttle, R. H.) 213-221 (Mouton,
The Hague, 1975).
12. Collard, M. & Aiello, L. C. Nature 404, 339-340 (2000).
| Article | PubMed |